• Research
    • VR in rehab
    • Sensory Imprecision
    • Affiliate Projects
  • Youtube
  • Videos
  • Blogs
  • About
  • Academic
  • Get in Touch
Daniel Harvie
  • Research
    • VR in rehab
    • Sensory Imprecision
    • Affiliate Projects
  • Youtube
  • Videos
  • Blogs
  • About
  • Academic
  • Get in Touch

Tactile hyperalgesia: new central mechanisms?

24/3/2020

0 Comments

 
Primary nociceptor activity is clearly not the only mechanism that can increase central sensitivity and pain. For example, certain cognitive and emotional states can also enhance pain and act centrally.
A recent proposal has suggested that associative learning mechanisms such as classical conditioning, may also contribute to the clinical features of sensitisation (hyperalgesia and allodynia) and the development of chronic pain (Moseley and Vlaeyen, 2015). The theory proposes that after repeated pairing of a non-nociceptive stimulus and a nociceptive stimulus, the non-nociceptive stimulus might come to elicit responses that are intrinsically associated with nociception—think of the bell causing salivation in Pavlov’s dogs.
That is, by association with nociception, non-nociceptive stimuli might acquire nociception-like properties.
A number of studies have investigated the nociception-like effects of pain-associated stimuli. For example, some studies have shown that after a visual stimulus is repeatedly paired with a noxious stimulus, the visual stimulus starts to activate the brain in nociception-like ways, and can enhance the pain evoked by other stimuli (Atlas et al., 2010; Diesch and Flor 2007; Jensen et al., 2014; Keltner et al., 2006; Jensen et al., 2012). Thus, pain-associated visual cues can cause temporary (centrally) sensitised states. Quite profound, I think. However, given that people rarely report pain in response to visual cues (dysynchiria being one exception), instead more often in response to touch and movement, we tested whether tactile cues would have the same effect (SJP paper here).
To test this we made a device that could deliver touch at different locations using small vibrating motors attached to the skin—the same ones that make your mobile phone vibrate. We paired touch at one location with a painful electrical stimulus, and touch at another location with a non-painful electrical stimulus. In the following test phase a painful electrical stimulus was perceived as more intense when paired with the pain-associated touch.
Notably, the effect of the pain-associated tactile cues was smaller than that shown for visual cues in the studies I mentioned previously. We are still trying to understand why this might be. It could be that an equally influential association may take more than the time of a short experiment to develop within the tactile system, which tends to be less dominant than the visual system.
Interestingly, the effect that we did see didn’t seem to depend on the expectation of pain that might be elicited by the tactile cue. That is, when we delivered the pain-associated tactile cue at virtually the same time as the noxious stimulus (such that there was no time to expect the stimulus) the effect remained the same—suggesting that the effect is not dependent on conscious processes.
This kind of associative central sensitisation might explain other findings, such as why visual signals of exaggerated movement can contribute to pain in people with persistent pain (Harvie et al. 2015), or why the colour red might increase sensitivity (Moseley & Arntz 2007)—because these signals might have a prior association with danger.
Perhaps in the future we will be treating pain by extinguishing associations between touch or movement and pain, or by associating touch or movement with less threatening experiences and information. Perhaps some are already pioneering this approach.
For further discussions and education subscribe to Daniel Harvie on Youtube
0 Comments



Leave a Reply.

    Author

    Dr Daniel Harvie is an NHMRC Early Career Research Fellow based at The Hopkins Centre in the Menzies Health Institute QLD at Griffith University. His main focus is the investigation of central nervous system contributions to persistent pain, and the development of brain-based treatments for preventing and treating persistent pain, including those that involve sensory re-training, virtual reality, and education.

    Categories

    All
    VR + Pain

    RSS Feed

    View my profile on LinkedIn
  • Research
    • VR in rehab
    • Sensory Imprecision
    • Affiliate Projects
  • Youtube
  • Videos
  • Blogs
  • About
  • Academic
  • Get in Touch